People in a rollercoaster or falling elevator feel the rapid descent, so why don't pilots in the cockpit?
$begingroup$
Judging from the CVR transcript, the AF447 pilots didn't realize for most of the plunge that they were stalled and rapidly losing altitude.
Why don't pilots in the cockpit get the same sensation of falling down as people in a rollercoaster or a falling elevator ? Both are situations in which you would unmistakeably know you are falling. (If you have never experienced a rollercoaster descent, believe me, it's noticeable ;-). So why and how does it differ for the pilot in the cockpit ?
medical aircraft-physics g-forces human-factors af447-accident
$endgroup$
|
show 4 more comments
$begingroup$
Judging from the CVR transcript, the AF447 pilots didn't realize for most of the plunge that they were stalled and rapidly losing altitude.
Why don't pilots in the cockpit get the same sensation of falling down as people in a rollercoaster or a falling elevator ? Both are situations in which you would unmistakeably know you are falling. (If you have never experienced a rollercoaster descent, believe me, it's noticeable ;-). So why and how does it differ for the pilot in the cockpit ?
medical aircraft-physics g-forces human-factors af447-accident
$endgroup$
16
$begingroup$
You seem to assume managing an abnormal situation is natural. It actually requires a lot of training to discard feelings and rely only on instrument indications. Pilots have flown inverted without knowing it, or think they are flying inverted, while they are just flying upright. It can happen at any time, to anyone, when external references are lost, and in particular at night. It's called spatial disorientation. The "the head-up illusion" (p7) for instance can give the feeling of pitching up while the aircraft is accelerating down.
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 12:16
9
$begingroup$
Your elevator comparison is biased, the elevator is falling at constant acceleration (compatible with our feeling of gravity), the problems arise when this acceleration either changes in direction, or in magnitude. The rollercoaster example is meaningful: Close your eyes, you won't know in which direction your are currently moving (and you will start to feel very bad).
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 15:08
14
$begingroup$
@summerrain I think you're conflating acceleration and velocity. Acceleration is the change in velocity over time. When you're in an object like an elevator, you feel a change in gravity from accelerating up or down. If you go up 100 stories in an elevator, you only feel a change in gravity while the elevator's velocity is changing (ie, starting or stopping). You don't feel anything while it's just moving up or down at a constant velocity.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:59
4
$begingroup$
"AF447's steady cruise flight prior to the stall" -- Have you read the accounts? The aircraft climbed 3000 feet first, while being tossed around by turbulence and by the pilot's overcorrections, then stalled. I've been in a plane that was stalled (for practice), and I've also experienced turbulence; the sensations I recall during the stall were mild compared to some of that turbulence.
$endgroup$
– David K
Jan 4 at 23:34
1
$begingroup$
Fly your pane like a rollercoaster and it will. A ride on the Vomit Comet costs $5k.
$endgroup$
– Mazura
Jan 5 at 6:51
|
show 4 more comments
$begingroup$
Judging from the CVR transcript, the AF447 pilots didn't realize for most of the plunge that they were stalled and rapidly losing altitude.
Why don't pilots in the cockpit get the same sensation of falling down as people in a rollercoaster or a falling elevator ? Both are situations in which you would unmistakeably know you are falling. (If you have never experienced a rollercoaster descent, believe me, it's noticeable ;-). So why and how does it differ for the pilot in the cockpit ?
medical aircraft-physics g-forces human-factors af447-accident
$endgroup$
Judging from the CVR transcript, the AF447 pilots didn't realize for most of the plunge that they were stalled and rapidly losing altitude.
Why don't pilots in the cockpit get the same sensation of falling down as people in a rollercoaster or a falling elevator ? Both are situations in which you would unmistakeably know you are falling. (If you have never experienced a rollercoaster descent, believe me, it's noticeable ;-). So why and how does it differ for the pilot in the cockpit ?
medical aircraft-physics g-forces human-factors af447-accident
medical aircraft-physics g-forces human-factors af447-accident
asked Jan 4 at 10:01
summerrainsummerrain
1,5831826
1,5831826
16
$begingroup$
You seem to assume managing an abnormal situation is natural. It actually requires a lot of training to discard feelings and rely only on instrument indications. Pilots have flown inverted without knowing it, or think they are flying inverted, while they are just flying upright. It can happen at any time, to anyone, when external references are lost, and in particular at night. It's called spatial disorientation. The "the head-up illusion" (p7) for instance can give the feeling of pitching up while the aircraft is accelerating down.
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 12:16
9
$begingroup$
Your elevator comparison is biased, the elevator is falling at constant acceleration (compatible with our feeling of gravity), the problems arise when this acceleration either changes in direction, or in magnitude. The rollercoaster example is meaningful: Close your eyes, you won't know in which direction your are currently moving (and you will start to feel very bad).
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 15:08
14
$begingroup$
@summerrain I think you're conflating acceleration and velocity. Acceleration is the change in velocity over time. When you're in an object like an elevator, you feel a change in gravity from accelerating up or down. If you go up 100 stories in an elevator, you only feel a change in gravity while the elevator's velocity is changing (ie, starting or stopping). You don't feel anything while it's just moving up or down at a constant velocity.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:59
4
$begingroup$
"AF447's steady cruise flight prior to the stall" -- Have you read the accounts? The aircraft climbed 3000 feet first, while being tossed around by turbulence and by the pilot's overcorrections, then stalled. I've been in a plane that was stalled (for practice), and I've also experienced turbulence; the sensations I recall during the stall were mild compared to some of that turbulence.
$endgroup$
– David K
Jan 4 at 23:34
1
$begingroup$
Fly your pane like a rollercoaster and it will. A ride on the Vomit Comet costs $5k.
$endgroup$
– Mazura
Jan 5 at 6:51
|
show 4 more comments
16
$begingroup$
You seem to assume managing an abnormal situation is natural. It actually requires a lot of training to discard feelings and rely only on instrument indications. Pilots have flown inverted without knowing it, or think they are flying inverted, while they are just flying upright. It can happen at any time, to anyone, when external references are lost, and in particular at night. It's called spatial disorientation. The "the head-up illusion" (p7) for instance can give the feeling of pitching up while the aircraft is accelerating down.
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 12:16
9
$begingroup$
Your elevator comparison is biased, the elevator is falling at constant acceleration (compatible with our feeling of gravity), the problems arise when this acceleration either changes in direction, or in magnitude. The rollercoaster example is meaningful: Close your eyes, you won't know in which direction your are currently moving (and you will start to feel very bad).
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 15:08
14
$begingroup$
@summerrain I think you're conflating acceleration and velocity. Acceleration is the change in velocity over time. When you're in an object like an elevator, you feel a change in gravity from accelerating up or down. If you go up 100 stories in an elevator, you only feel a change in gravity while the elevator's velocity is changing (ie, starting or stopping). You don't feel anything while it's just moving up or down at a constant velocity.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:59
4
$begingroup$
"AF447's steady cruise flight prior to the stall" -- Have you read the accounts? The aircraft climbed 3000 feet first, while being tossed around by turbulence and by the pilot's overcorrections, then stalled. I've been in a plane that was stalled (for practice), and I've also experienced turbulence; the sensations I recall during the stall were mild compared to some of that turbulence.
$endgroup$
– David K
Jan 4 at 23:34
1
$begingroup$
Fly your pane like a rollercoaster and it will. A ride on the Vomit Comet costs $5k.
$endgroup$
– Mazura
Jan 5 at 6:51
16
16
$begingroup$
You seem to assume managing an abnormal situation is natural. It actually requires a lot of training to discard feelings and rely only on instrument indications. Pilots have flown inverted without knowing it, or think they are flying inverted, while they are just flying upright. It can happen at any time, to anyone, when external references are lost, and in particular at night. It's called spatial disorientation. The "the head-up illusion" (p7) for instance can give the feeling of pitching up while the aircraft is accelerating down.
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 12:16
$begingroup$
You seem to assume managing an abnormal situation is natural. It actually requires a lot of training to discard feelings and rely only on instrument indications. Pilots have flown inverted without knowing it, or think they are flying inverted, while they are just flying upright. It can happen at any time, to anyone, when external references are lost, and in particular at night. It's called spatial disorientation. The "the head-up illusion" (p7) for instance can give the feeling of pitching up while the aircraft is accelerating down.
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 12:16
9
9
$begingroup$
Your elevator comparison is biased, the elevator is falling at constant acceleration (compatible with our feeling of gravity), the problems arise when this acceleration either changes in direction, or in magnitude. The rollercoaster example is meaningful: Close your eyes, you won't know in which direction your are currently moving (and you will start to feel very bad).
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 15:08
$begingroup$
Your elevator comparison is biased, the elevator is falling at constant acceleration (compatible with our feeling of gravity), the problems arise when this acceleration either changes in direction, or in magnitude. The rollercoaster example is meaningful: Close your eyes, you won't know in which direction your are currently moving (and you will start to feel very bad).
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 15:08
14
14
$begingroup$
@summerrain I think you're conflating acceleration and velocity. Acceleration is the change in velocity over time. When you're in an object like an elevator, you feel a change in gravity from accelerating up or down. If you go up 100 stories in an elevator, you only feel a change in gravity while the elevator's velocity is changing (ie, starting or stopping). You don't feel anything while it's just moving up or down at a constant velocity.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:59
$begingroup$
@summerrain I think you're conflating acceleration and velocity. Acceleration is the change in velocity over time. When you're in an object like an elevator, you feel a change in gravity from accelerating up or down. If you go up 100 stories in an elevator, you only feel a change in gravity while the elevator's velocity is changing (ie, starting or stopping). You don't feel anything while it's just moving up or down at a constant velocity.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:59
4
4
$begingroup$
"AF447's steady cruise flight prior to the stall" -- Have you read the accounts? The aircraft climbed 3000 feet first, while being tossed around by turbulence and by the pilot's overcorrections, then stalled. I've been in a plane that was stalled (for practice), and I've also experienced turbulence; the sensations I recall during the stall were mild compared to some of that turbulence.
$endgroup$
– David K
Jan 4 at 23:34
$begingroup$
"AF447's steady cruise flight prior to the stall" -- Have you read the accounts? The aircraft climbed 3000 feet first, while being tossed around by turbulence and by the pilot's overcorrections, then stalled. I've been in a plane that was stalled (for practice), and I've also experienced turbulence; the sensations I recall during the stall were mild compared to some of that turbulence.
$endgroup$
– David K
Jan 4 at 23:34
1
1
$begingroup$
Fly your pane like a rollercoaster and it will. A ride on the Vomit Comet costs $5k.
$endgroup$
– Mazura
Jan 5 at 6:51
$begingroup$
Fly your pane like a rollercoaster and it will. A ride on the Vomit Comet costs $5k.
$endgroup$
– Mazura
Jan 5 at 6:51
|
show 4 more comments
5 Answers
5
active
oldest
votes
$begingroup$
You only feel the acceleration downward. In roller-coaster this sensation is maximized for maximum thrill. A stall isn't instant: some parts of the wing can be stalled while the rest still provides proper lift. Once the airplane is near or at terminal velocity in a stall it will feel no different from regular straight and level flight.
The onset of the stall was paired with some turbulence which would mask the downward acceleration.
Pilots are also trained to ignore their own sensations because they can be misleading leading to spatial disorientation. You can be in a spiraling dive and still feel as if you are flying straight and level.
$endgroup$
$begingroup$
The aircraft encountered vertical accelerations between +0.5G and +1.7G.
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 10:57
9
$begingroup$
+1 for mentioning the spiral dive. In a 60 degree steep turn you can be pulling +2g and not notice
$endgroup$
– Dave Gremlin
Jan 4 at 11:15
7
$begingroup$
There is also the lack of wind. Plus since the op mentioned elevators, might be worth pointing out you only feel the initial acceleration, after that you only feel vibration .
$endgroup$
– Notts90
Jan 4 at 11:57
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain just because they experience momentary accelerations in that range doesn't mean they felt a constant "falling" acceleration. I don't know what the limit is, but you can be descending at a constant rate and not notice the less-than-1G especially if you are sitting. Close to zero or negative G's and of course you will notice because you will float, but as long as there is some constant positive G, you might not realize you are falling.
$endgroup$
– JPhi1618
Jan 4 at 16:38
6
$begingroup$
I think this answer is onto the right track by mentioning that acceleration is what is felt, but fails to get into the areas of constant velocity or equilibrium which is really what is masking the sensation of descent.
$endgroup$
– 2NinerRomeo
Jan 5 at 0:05
|
show 3 more comments
$begingroup$
You only feel the plunging sensation during the initial downward acceleration. Once stabilized at a constant rate of descent, things feel normal again. The other thing is, the amount of vertical acceleration from a stall type maneuver does not result in 0 or negative G, just less than 1. You'll feel getting light in the seat, you won't lift right out and if you are being assaulted by several stimuli at once, as this crew was, you won't even notice it.
You can easily make an airplane go 0 G or negative by getting some speed and pushing hard. It's called a "bunt". I can make a pencil float in front of my face for about 3-5 seconds in my airplane (push a little bit too hard, and all the crap on the floor ends up on my canopy) If you are able to nail the 0G point precisely, it's exactly the same sensation as being in a space craft, although short lived. A jet going 300 kt can keep it up for 30 sec, which is how they train astronauts for weightlessness.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
You are forgetting one large difference between an aircraft moving through the air (still generating some lift), and an object in free-fall. Constant velocity in a gravity well (such as on the Earth) will cause you to still experience the acceleration due to gravity. It's when you're accelerating that you feel different.
Think of it this way - imagine driving over a hill in a car with cruise control. It's only as you're cresting the hill do you feel "weightless", because you are accelerating downward. On the way down the hill (assuming cruise control maintains speed downhill in this car), you will experience gravity normally.
$endgroup$
$begingroup$
I don't think it's correct that you feel speed changes only. Falling at a constant rate, you'll notice you are lighter – especially once you start floating :-) as JPhi1618 mentioned
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 17:28
15
$begingroup$
@summerrain That's not true. If you're in a plane falling at a constant rate, you'd feel the exact same weight as normal. What you're probably thinking of is the 'vomit comet' planes, that astronauts use to train in weightlessness. Those planes aren't falling at a constant rate, they're accelerating towards the ground at 9.81 m/s^2. Since everything inside the plane is also accelerating towards the ground at the same rate due to the normal force of gravity, the people inside feel weightless. To be falling at a constant rate, the vertical acceleration would need to be zero.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:48
1
$begingroup$
@summerrain Falling at any constant rate, even zero, means that the forces applied to you in all directions sum to zero. So precisely the same forces on you (those that, including gravity, sum to zero) can mean you are falling at any rate or not falling at all. This is one of the reasons pilots are specifically trained not to try to judge aircraft motion based on subjective force experiences -- flying by the seat of your pants is not encouraged.
$endgroup$
– David Schwartz
Jan 4 at 19:04
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain Wrong. Falling at constant rate you will not only not feel lighter but if you were to weigh yourself on a scale your weight will not be lighter. The scale will only register you as lighter when speed changes. Weight is caused by acceleration, not speed. That's why gravity's physical unit is acceleration (m/s/s) not speed (m/s)
$endgroup$
– slebetman
Jan 5 at 4:50
add a comment |
$begingroup$
A few things.
That accident took place at night and in IMC while the aircraft was passing through a storm which I suspect had quite a bit of turbulence in its core. This would most likely have caused enough vestibular disturbance for the crew to make the acceleration towards the deck go unnoticeable.
Secondly the crew was on an IFR flight which means flying by instruments in conditions and specifically trained to rely on cockpit instruments for aircraft orientation and not vestibular senses. Doing otherwise will get you killed in any other situation. Once the aircraft reached its peak descent rate in dark night over water and with the air data sensors iced over, preventing the flight control computers from receiving accurate air data, there was no perceptible means for the crew to sense they were in danger until it was all over.
Pilots do experience the vestibular sense of accelerating as occupants in a carnival ride do - try flying aerobatics some time and yes, it’s quite noticeable when pulling negative Gs, etc. it’s just not a good standard to use to judge movement in an airplane by and without visual references outside of the aircraft it is difficult and very dangerous to use vestibular senses to accurately assess aircraft attitude from.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
Sensation of motion can come through three of the physical senses. Each of the three senses is successively unavailable as we move through the examples of an amusement park ride, the plummeting elevator, and AF447.
Vision
This sense is fairly well understood, so I'll limit this bullet point to pointing out that the visual sense is correlated in the human brain to the vestibular sense, and on the amusement park ride, it is correlated to the vestibular sense. In cases where those two senses are decoupled, it is common for nausea to result. In the elevator and airplane in instrument conditions, a visual sense grounding you to a world reference frame is denied. This is one of the senses which can detect unaccelerated motion, more on "unaccelerated" below.
Touch/Kinesthetic
Here we have information which comes in through our sense of touch, primarily through nerve endings in the skin. The roller coaster comes through here again by exposing you to the elements giving you a "rush of wind" in your face as you move through what may be a stationary mass of air. In the closed environments of the elevator and cockpit, this is again denied.
Vestibular/Inertial
Here is where things get interesting. For the purposes of this discussion, I'll add to the vestibular senses the "seat of the pants", "pit of the stomach" and other bodily means of detecting acceleration. Several other answers have pointed out that all of these senses are sensitive not to velocity but to acceleration, which high school physics reminds us is the the time rate of change, or derivative, of velocity. This means that if velocity is not changing we are not going to have a sensation of motion, although in a gravity field, we may have some sense of orientation (you can have a sense of hanging upside down or laying sideways). There is no difference to these senses between sitting on the ground with the engines running, cruising at 500 knots in level flight, or being in a 500,1000, or 3000 foot per minute descent. As long as velocity is not changing, these senses cannot tell the difference. This is the problem with AF447, No visual, no touch, and in unaccelerated descent, no vestibular sense.
If the airdata instruments are offline, there is nothing to indicate the descent until the cabin pressure starts to increase, putting some pressure on your eardrums. That also won't happen until the aircraft descends to a point where the outside air pressure increases to a point above the cabin pressure then continues to build to a point that you notice. They were doomed.
There is another problem, even with detecting acceleration. The problem is connected to the human brain's remarkable ability to filter out stimuli. A normal human brain is capable of adjusting to the tint of your sunglasses, ignoring conversations in a crowded room, and is very good at not notifying the conscious thought center of the pressure your elastic socks are putting on your ankles. Indeed, it can even lose track of constant accelerations. If you live near a playa, you can do a blind test by driving your car out and enter a gentle turn giving a tenth of a "g" of lateral acceleration. Have someone else do the driving. I am here to tell you that the resulting sensation can be filtered out. Even our sense of acceleration can be lost under constant acceleration. In physics, there is yet another level of derivative, the time rate of change of acceleration, called "jerk", which is harder to not detect.
For more information on the brain's filtering capability, look up the "Reticular Activating System". Understanding it can help you be more safe, and also help you reach your goals.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
Your Answer
StackExchange.ifUsing("editor", function () {
return StackExchange.using("mathjaxEditing", function () {
StackExchange.MarkdownEditor.creationCallbacks.add(function (editor, postfix) {
StackExchange.mathjaxEditing.prepareWmdForMathJax(editor, postfix, [["$", "$"], ["\\(","\\)"]]);
});
});
}, "mathjax-editing");
StackExchange.ready(function() {
var channelOptions = {
tags: "".split(" "),
id: "528"
};
initTagRenderer("".split(" "), "".split(" "), channelOptions);
StackExchange.using("externalEditor", function() {
// Have to fire editor after snippets, if snippets enabled
if (StackExchange.settings.snippets.snippetsEnabled) {
StackExchange.using("snippets", function() {
createEditor();
});
}
else {
createEditor();
}
});
function createEditor() {
StackExchange.prepareEditor({
heartbeatType: 'answer',
autoActivateHeartbeat: false,
convertImagesToLinks: false,
noModals: true,
showLowRepImageUploadWarning: true,
reputationToPostImages: null,
bindNavPrevention: true,
postfix: "",
imageUploader: {
brandingHtml: "Powered by u003ca class="icon-imgur-white" href="https://imgur.com/"u003eu003c/au003e",
contentPolicyHtml: "User contributions licensed under u003ca href="https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0/"u003ecc by-sa 3.0 with attribution requiredu003c/au003e u003ca href="https://stackoverflow.com/legal/content-policy"u003e(content policy)u003c/au003e",
allowUrls: true
},
noCode: true, onDemand: true,
discardSelector: ".discard-answer"
,immediatelyShowMarkdownHelp:true
});
}
});
Sign up or log in
StackExchange.ready(function () {
StackExchange.helpers.onClickDraftSave('#login-link');
});
Sign up using Google
Sign up using Facebook
Sign up using Email and Password
Post as a guest
Required, but never shown
StackExchange.ready(
function () {
StackExchange.openid.initPostLogin('.new-post-login', 'https%3a%2f%2faviation.stackexchange.com%2fquestions%2f58700%2fpeople-in-a-rollercoaster-or-falling-elevator-feel-the-rapid-descent-so-why-don%23new-answer', 'question_page');
}
);
Post as a guest
Required, but never shown
5 Answers
5
active
oldest
votes
5 Answers
5
active
oldest
votes
active
oldest
votes
active
oldest
votes
$begingroup$
You only feel the acceleration downward. In roller-coaster this sensation is maximized for maximum thrill. A stall isn't instant: some parts of the wing can be stalled while the rest still provides proper lift. Once the airplane is near or at terminal velocity in a stall it will feel no different from regular straight and level flight.
The onset of the stall was paired with some turbulence which would mask the downward acceleration.
Pilots are also trained to ignore their own sensations because they can be misleading leading to spatial disorientation. You can be in a spiraling dive and still feel as if you are flying straight and level.
$endgroup$
$begingroup$
The aircraft encountered vertical accelerations between +0.5G and +1.7G.
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 10:57
9
$begingroup$
+1 for mentioning the spiral dive. In a 60 degree steep turn you can be pulling +2g and not notice
$endgroup$
– Dave Gremlin
Jan 4 at 11:15
7
$begingroup$
There is also the lack of wind. Plus since the op mentioned elevators, might be worth pointing out you only feel the initial acceleration, after that you only feel vibration .
$endgroup$
– Notts90
Jan 4 at 11:57
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain just because they experience momentary accelerations in that range doesn't mean they felt a constant "falling" acceleration. I don't know what the limit is, but you can be descending at a constant rate and not notice the less-than-1G especially if you are sitting. Close to zero or negative G's and of course you will notice because you will float, but as long as there is some constant positive G, you might not realize you are falling.
$endgroup$
– JPhi1618
Jan 4 at 16:38
6
$begingroup$
I think this answer is onto the right track by mentioning that acceleration is what is felt, but fails to get into the areas of constant velocity or equilibrium which is really what is masking the sensation of descent.
$endgroup$
– 2NinerRomeo
Jan 5 at 0:05
|
show 3 more comments
$begingroup$
You only feel the acceleration downward. In roller-coaster this sensation is maximized for maximum thrill. A stall isn't instant: some parts of the wing can be stalled while the rest still provides proper lift. Once the airplane is near or at terminal velocity in a stall it will feel no different from regular straight and level flight.
The onset of the stall was paired with some turbulence which would mask the downward acceleration.
Pilots are also trained to ignore their own sensations because they can be misleading leading to spatial disorientation. You can be in a spiraling dive and still feel as if you are flying straight and level.
$endgroup$
$begingroup$
The aircraft encountered vertical accelerations between +0.5G and +1.7G.
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 10:57
9
$begingroup$
+1 for mentioning the spiral dive. In a 60 degree steep turn you can be pulling +2g and not notice
$endgroup$
– Dave Gremlin
Jan 4 at 11:15
7
$begingroup$
There is also the lack of wind. Plus since the op mentioned elevators, might be worth pointing out you only feel the initial acceleration, after that you only feel vibration .
$endgroup$
– Notts90
Jan 4 at 11:57
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain just because they experience momentary accelerations in that range doesn't mean they felt a constant "falling" acceleration. I don't know what the limit is, but you can be descending at a constant rate and not notice the less-than-1G especially if you are sitting. Close to zero or negative G's and of course you will notice because you will float, but as long as there is some constant positive G, you might not realize you are falling.
$endgroup$
– JPhi1618
Jan 4 at 16:38
6
$begingroup$
I think this answer is onto the right track by mentioning that acceleration is what is felt, but fails to get into the areas of constant velocity or equilibrium which is really what is masking the sensation of descent.
$endgroup$
– 2NinerRomeo
Jan 5 at 0:05
|
show 3 more comments
$begingroup$
You only feel the acceleration downward. In roller-coaster this sensation is maximized for maximum thrill. A stall isn't instant: some parts of the wing can be stalled while the rest still provides proper lift. Once the airplane is near or at terminal velocity in a stall it will feel no different from regular straight and level flight.
The onset of the stall was paired with some turbulence which would mask the downward acceleration.
Pilots are also trained to ignore their own sensations because they can be misleading leading to spatial disorientation. You can be in a spiraling dive and still feel as if you are flying straight and level.
$endgroup$
You only feel the acceleration downward. In roller-coaster this sensation is maximized for maximum thrill. A stall isn't instant: some parts of the wing can be stalled while the rest still provides proper lift. Once the airplane is near or at terminal velocity in a stall it will feel no different from regular straight and level flight.
The onset of the stall was paired with some turbulence which would mask the downward acceleration.
Pilots are also trained to ignore their own sensations because they can be misleading leading to spatial disorientation. You can be in a spiraling dive and still feel as if you are flying straight and level.
edited Jan 6 at 16:29
psmears
26124
26124
answered Jan 4 at 10:28
ratchet freakratchet freak
24k467130
24k467130
$begingroup$
The aircraft encountered vertical accelerations between +0.5G and +1.7G.
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 10:57
9
$begingroup$
+1 for mentioning the spiral dive. In a 60 degree steep turn you can be pulling +2g and not notice
$endgroup$
– Dave Gremlin
Jan 4 at 11:15
7
$begingroup$
There is also the lack of wind. Plus since the op mentioned elevators, might be worth pointing out you only feel the initial acceleration, after that you only feel vibration .
$endgroup$
– Notts90
Jan 4 at 11:57
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain just because they experience momentary accelerations in that range doesn't mean they felt a constant "falling" acceleration. I don't know what the limit is, but you can be descending at a constant rate and not notice the less-than-1G especially if you are sitting. Close to zero or negative G's and of course you will notice because you will float, but as long as there is some constant positive G, you might not realize you are falling.
$endgroup$
– JPhi1618
Jan 4 at 16:38
6
$begingroup$
I think this answer is onto the right track by mentioning that acceleration is what is felt, but fails to get into the areas of constant velocity or equilibrium which is really what is masking the sensation of descent.
$endgroup$
– 2NinerRomeo
Jan 5 at 0:05
|
show 3 more comments
$begingroup$
The aircraft encountered vertical accelerations between +0.5G and +1.7G.
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 10:57
9
$begingroup$
+1 for mentioning the spiral dive. In a 60 degree steep turn you can be pulling +2g and not notice
$endgroup$
– Dave Gremlin
Jan 4 at 11:15
7
$begingroup$
There is also the lack of wind. Plus since the op mentioned elevators, might be worth pointing out you only feel the initial acceleration, after that you only feel vibration .
$endgroup$
– Notts90
Jan 4 at 11:57
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain just because they experience momentary accelerations in that range doesn't mean they felt a constant "falling" acceleration. I don't know what the limit is, but you can be descending at a constant rate and not notice the less-than-1G especially if you are sitting. Close to zero or negative G's and of course you will notice because you will float, but as long as there is some constant positive G, you might not realize you are falling.
$endgroup$
– JPhi1618
Jan 4 at 16:38
6
$begingroup$
I think this answer is onto the right track by mentioning that acceleration is what is felt, but fails to get into the areas of constant velocity or equilibrium which is really what is masking the sensation of descent.
$endgroup$
– 2NinerRomeo
Jan 5 at 0:05
$begingroup$
The aircraft encountered vertical accelerations between +0.5G and +1.7G.
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 10:57
$begingroup$
The aircraft encountered vertical accelerations between +0.5G and +1.7G.
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 10:57
9
9
$begingroup$
+1 for mentioning the spiral dive. In a 60 degree steep turn you can be pulling +2g and not notice
$endgroup$
– Dave Gremlin
Jan 4 at 11:15
$begingroup$
+1 for mentioning the spiral dive. In a 60 degree steep turn you can be pulling +2g and not notice
$endgroup$
– Dave Gremlin
Jan 4 at 11:15
7
7
$begingroup$
There is also the lack of wind. Plus since the op mentioned elevators, might be worth pointing out you only feel the initial acceleration, after that you only feel vibration .
$endgroup$
– Notts90
Jan 4 at 11:57
$begingroup$
There is also the lack of wind. Plus since the op mentioned elevators, might be worth pointing out you only feel the initial acceleration, after that you only feel vibration .
$endgroup$
– Notts90
Jan 4 at 11:57
3
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain just because they experience momentary accelerations in that range doesn't mean they felt a constant "falling" acceleration. I don't know what the limit is, but you can be descending at a constant rate and not notice the less-than-1G especially if you are sitting. Close to zero or negative G's and of course you will notice because you will float, but as long as there is some constant positive G, you might not realize you are falling.
$endgroup$
– JPhi1618
Jan 4 at 16:38
$begingroup$
@summerrain just because they experience momentary accelerations in that range doesn't mean they felt a constant "falling" acceleration. I don't know what the limit is, but you can be descending at a constant rate and not notice the less-than-1G especially if you are sitting. Close to zero or negative G's and of course you will notice because you will float, but as long as there is some constant positive G, you might not realize you are falling.
$endgroup$
– JPhi1618
Jan 4 at 16:38
6
6
$begingroup$
I think this answer is onto the right track by mentioning that acceleration is what is felt, but fails to get into the areas of constant velocity or equilibrium which is really what is masking the sensation of descent.
$endgroup$
– 2NinerRomeo
Jan 5 at 0:05
$begingroup$
I think this answer is onto the right track by mentioning that acceleration is what is felt, but fails to get into the areas of constant velocity or equilibrium which is really what is masking the sensation of descent.
$endgroup$
– 2NinerRomeo
Jan 5 at 0:05
|
show 3 more comments
$begingroup$
You only feel the plunging sensation during the initial downward acceleration. Once stabilized at a constant rate of descent, things feel normal again. The other thing is, the amount of vertical acceleration from a stall type maneuver does not result in 0 or negative G, just less than 1. You'll feel getting light in the seat, you won't lift right out and if you are being assaulted by several stimuli at once, as this crew was, you won't even notice it.
You can easily make an airplane go 0 G or negative by getting some speed and pushing hard. It's called a "bunt". I can make a pencil float in front of my face for about 3-5 seconds in my airplane (push a little bit too hard, and all the crap on the floor ends up on my canopy) If you are able to nail the 0G point precisely, it's exactly the same sensation as being in a space craft, although short lived. A jet going 300 kt can keep it up for 30 sec, which is how they train astronauts for weightlessness.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
You only feel the plunging sensation during the initial downward acceleration. Once stabilized at a constant rate of descent, things feel normal again. The other thing is, the amount of vertical acceleration from a stall type maneuver does not result in 0 or negative G, just less than 1. You'll feel getting light in the seat, you won't lift right out and if you are being assaulted by several stimuli at once, as this crew was, you won't even notice it.
You can easily make an airplane go 0 G or negative by getting some speed and pushing hard. It's called a "bunt". I can make a pencil float in front of my face for about 3-5 seconds in my airplane (push a little bit too hard, and all the crap on the floor ends up on my canopy) If you are able to nail the 0G point precisely, it's exactly the same sensation as being in a space craft, although short lived. A jet going 300 kt can keep it up for 30 sec, which is how they train astronauts for weightlessness.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
You only feel the plunging sensation during the initial downward acceleration. Once stabilized at a constant rate of descent, things feel normal again. The other thing is, the amount of vertical acceleration from a stall type maneuver does not result in 0 or negative G, just less than 1. You'll feel getting light in the seat, you won't lift right out and if you are being assaulted by several stimuli at once, as this crew was, you won't even notice it.
You can easily make an airplane go 0 G or negative by getting some speed and pushing hard. It's called a "bunt". I can make a pencil float in front of my face for about 3-5 seconds in my airplane (push a little bit too hard, and all the crap on the floor ends up on my canopy) If you are able to nail the 0G point precisely, it's exactly the same sensation as being in a space craft, although short lived. A jet going 300 kt can keep it up for 30 sec, which is how they train astronauts for weightlessness.
$endgroup$
You only feel the plunging sensation during the initial downward acceleration. Once stabilized at a constant rate of descent, things feel normal again. The other thing is, the amount of vertical acceleration from a stall type maneuver does not result in 0 or negative G, just less than 1. You'll feel getting light in the seat, you won't lift right out and if you are being assaulted by several stimuli at once, as this crew was, you won't even notice it.
You can easily make an airplane go 0 G or negative by getting some speed and pushing hard. It's called a "bunt". I can make a pencil float in front of my face for about 3-5 seconds in my airplane (push a little bit too hard, and all the crap on the floor ends up on my canopy) If you are able to nail the 0G point precisely, it's exactly the same sensation as being in a space craft, although short lived. A jet going 300 kt can keep it up for 30 sec, which is how they train astronauts for weightlessness.
answered Jan 4 at 19:22
John KJohn K
24.5k13674
24.5k13674
add a comment |
add a comment |
$begingroup$
You are forgetting one large difference between an aircraft moving through the air (still generating some lift), and an object in free-fall. Constant velocity in a gravity well (such as on the Earth) will cause you to still experience the acceleration due to gravity. It's when you're accelerating that you feel different.
Think of it this way - imagine driving over a hill in a car with cruise control. It's only as you're cresting the hill do you feel "weightless", because you are accelerating downward. On the way down the hill (assuming cruise control maintains speed downhill in this car), you will experience gravity normally.
$endgroup$
$begingroup$
I don't think it's correct that you feel speed changes only. Falling at a constant rate, you'll notice you are lighter – especially once you start floating :-) as JPhi1618 mentioned
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 17:28
15
$begingroup$
@summerrain That's not true. If you're in a plane falling at a constant rate, you'd feel the exact same weight as normal. What you're probably thinking of is the 'vomit comet' planes, that astronauts use to train in weightlessness. Those planes aren't falling at a constant rate, they're accelerating towards the ground at 9.81 m/s^2. Since everything inside the plane is also accelerating towards the ground at the same rate due to the normal force of gravity, the people inside feel weightless. To be falling at a constant rate, the vertical acceleration would need to be zero.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:48
1
$begingroup$
@summerrain Falling at any constant rate, even zero, means that the forces applied to you in all directions sum to zero. So precisely the same forces on you (those that, including gravity, sum to zero) can mean you are falling at any rate or not falling at all. This is one of the reasons pilots are specifically trained not to try to judge aircraft motion based on subjective force experiences -- flying by the seat of your pants is not encouraged.
$endgroup$
– David Schwartz
Jan 4 at 19:04
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain Wrong. Falling at constant rate you will not only not feel lighter but if you were to weigh yourself on a scale your weight will not be lighter. The scale will only register you as lighter when speed changes. Weight is caused by acceleration, not speed. That's why gravity's physical unit is acceleration (m/s/s) not speed (m/s)
$endgroup$
– slebetman
Jan 5 at 4:50
add a comment |
$begingroup$
You are forgetting one large difference between an aircraft moving through the air (still generating some lift), and an object in free-fall. Constant velocity in a gravity well (such as on the Earth) will cause you to still experience the acceleration due to gravity. It's when you're accelerating that you feel different.
Think of it this way - imagine driving over a hill in a car with cruise control. It's only as you're cresting the hill do you feel "weightless", because you are accelerating downward. On the way down the hill (assuming cruise control maintains speed downhill in this car), you will experience gravity normally.
$endgroup$
$begingroup$
I don't think it's correct that you feel speed changes only. Falling at a constant rate, you'll notice you are lighter – especially once you start floating :-) as JPhi1618 mentioned
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 17:28
15
$begingroup$
@summerrain That's not true. If you're in a plane falling at a constant rate, you'd feel the exact same weight as normal. What you're probably thinking of is the 'vomit comet' planes, that astronauts use to train in weightlessness. Those planes aren't falling at a constant rate, they're accelerating towards the ground at 9.81 m/s^2. Since everything inside the plane is also accelerating towards the ground at the same rate due to the normal force of gravity, the people inside feel weightless. To be falling at a constant rate, the vertical acceleration would need to be zero.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:48
1
$begingroup$
@summerrain Falling at any constant rate, even zero, means that the forces applied to you in all directions sum to zero. So precisely the same forces on you (those that, including gravity, sum to zero) can mean you are falling at any rate or not falling at all. This is one of the reasons pilots are specifically trained not to try to judge aircraft motion based on subjective force experiences -- flying by the seat of your pants is not encouraged.
$endgroup$
– David Schwartz
Jan 4 at 19:04
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain Wrong. Falling at constant rate you will not only not feel lighter but if you were to weigh yourself on a scale your weight will not be lighter. The scale will only register you as lighter when speed changes. Weight is caused by acceleration, not speed. That's why gravity's physical unit is acceleration (m/s/s) not speed (m/s)
$endgroup$
– slebetman
Jan 5 at 4:50
add a comment |
$begingroup$
You are forgetting one large difference between an aircraft moving through the air (still generating some lift), and an object in free-fall. Constant velocity in a gravity well (such as on the Earth) will cause you to still experience the acceleration due to gravity. It's when you're accelerating that you feel different.
Think of it this way - imagine driving over a hill in a car with cruise control. It's only as you're cresting the hill do you feel "weightless", because you are accelerating downward. On the way down the hill (assuming cruise control maintains speed downhill in this car), you will experience gravity normally.
$endgroup$
You are forgetting one large difference between an aircraft moving through the air (still generating some lift), and an object in free-fall. Constant velocity in a gravity well (such as on the Earth) will cause you to still experience the acceleration due to gravity. It's when you're accelerating that you feel different.
Think of it this way - imagine driving over a hill in a car with cruise control. It's only as you're cresting the hill do you feel "weightless", because you are accelerating downward. On the way down the hill (assuming cruise control maintains speed downhill in this car), you will experience gravity normally.
answered Jan 4 at 16:44
TyzoidTyzoid
1812
1812
$begingroup$
I don't think it's correct that you feel speed changes only. Falling at a constant rate, you'll notice you are lighter – especially once you start floating :-) as JPhi1618 mentioned
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 17:28
15
$begingroup$
@summerrain That's not true. If you're in a plane falling at a constant rate, you'd feel the exact same weight as normal. What you're probably thinking of is the 'vomit comet' planes, that astronauts use to train in weightlessness. Those planes aren't falling at a constant rate, they're accelerating towards the ground at 9.81 m/s^2. Since everything inside the plane is also accelerating towards the ground at the same rate due to the normal force of gravity, the people inside feel weightless. To be falling at a constant rate, the vertical acceleration would need to be zero.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:48
1
$begingroup$
@summerrain Falling at any constant rate, even zero, means that the forces applied to you in all directions sum to zero. So precisely the same forces on you (those that, including gravity, sum to zero) can mean you are falling at any rate or not falling at all. This is one of the reasons pilots are specifically trained not to try to judge aircraft motion based on subjective force experiences -- flying by the seat of your pants is not encouraged.
$endgroup$
– David Schwartz
Jan 4 at 19:04
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain Wrong. Falling at constant rate you will not only not feel lighter but if you were to weigh yourself on a scale your weight will not be lighter. The scale will only register you as lighter when speed changes. Weight is caused by acceleration, not speed. That's why gravity's physical unit is acceleration (m/s/s) not speed (m/s)
$endgroup$
– slebetman
Jan 5 at 4:50
add a comment |
$begingroup$
I don't think it's correct that you feel speed changes only. Falling at a constant rate, you'll notice you are lighter – especially once you start floating :-) as JPhi1618 mentioned
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 17:28
15
$begingroup$
@summerrain That's not true. If you're in a plane falling at a constant rate, you'd feel the exact same weight as normal. What you're probably thinking of is the 'vomit comet' planes, that astronauts use to train in weightlessness. Those planes aren't falling at a constant rate, they're accelerating towards the ground at 9.81 m/s^2. Since everything inside the plane is also accelerating towards the ground at the same rate due to the normal force of gravity, the people inside feel weightless. To be falling at a constant rate, the vertical acceleration would need to be zero.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:48
1
$begingroup$
@summerrain Falling at any constant rate, even zero, means that the forces applied to you in all directions sum to zero. So precisely the same forces on you (those that, including gravity, sum to zero) can mean you are falling at any rate or not falling at all. This is one of the reasons pilots are specifically trained not to try to judge aircraft motion based on subjective force experiences -- flying by the seat of your pants is not encouraged.
$endgroup$
– David Schwartz
Jan 4 at 19:04
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain Wrong. Falling at constant rate you will not only not feel lighter but if you were to weigh yourself on a scale your weight will not be lighter. The scale will only register you as lighter when speed changes. Weight is caused by acceleration, not speed. That's why gravity's physical unit is acceleration (m/s/s) not speed (m/s)
$endgroup$
– slebetman
Jan 5 at 4:50
$begingroup$
I don't think it's correct that you feel speed changes only. Falling at a constant rate, you'll notice you are lighter – especially once you start floating :-) as JPhi1618 mentioned
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 17:28
$begingroup$
I don't think it's correct that you feel speed changes only. Falling at a constant rate, you'll notice you are lighter – especially once you start floating :-) as JPhi1618 mentioned
$endgroup$
– summerrain
Jan 4 at 17:28
15
15
$begingroup$
@summerrain That's not true. If you're in a plane falling at a constant rate, you'd feel the exact same weight as normal. What you're probably thinking of is the 'vomit comet' planes, that astronauts use to train in weightlessness. Those planes aren't falling at a constant rate, they're accelerating towards the ground at 9.81 m/s^2. Since everything inside the plane is also accelerating towards the ground at the same rate due to the normal force of gravity, the people inside feel weightless. To be falling at a constant rate, the vertical acceleration would need to be zero.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:48
$begingroup$
@summerrain That's not true. If you're in a plane falling at a constant rate, you'd feel the exact same weight as normal. What you're probably thinking of is the 'vomit comet' planes, that astronauts use to train in weightlessness. Those planes aren't falling at a constant rate, they're accelerating towards the ground at 9.81 m/s^2. Since everything inside the plane is also accelerating towards the ground at the same rate due to the normal force of gravity, the people inside feel weightless. To be falling at a constant rate, the vertical acceleration would need to be zero.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:48
1
1
$begingroup$
@summerrain Falling at any constant rate, even zero, means that the forces applied to you in all directions sum to zero. So precisely the same forces on you (those that, including gravity, sum to zero) can mean you are falling at any rate or not falling at all. This is one of the reasons pilots are specifically trained not to try to judge aircraft motion based on subjective force experiences -- flying by the seat of your pants is not encouraged.
$endgroup$
– David Schwartz
Jan 4 at 19:04
$begingroup$
@summerrain Falling at any constant rate, even zero, means that the forces applied to you in all directions sum to zero. So precisely the same forces on you (those that, including gravity, sum to zero) can mean you are falling at any rate or not falling at all. This is one of the reasons pilots are specifically trained not to try to judge aircraft motion based on subjective force experiences -- flying by the seat of your pants is not encouraged.
$endgroup$
– David Schwartz
Jan 4 at 19:04
3
3
$begingroup$
@summerrain Wrong. Falling at constant rate you will not only not feel lighter but if you were to weigh yourself on a scale your weight will not be lighter. The scale will only register you as lighter when speed changes. Weight is caused by acceleration, not speed. That's why gravity's physical unit is acceleration (m/s/s) not speed (m/s)
$endgroup$
– slebetman
Jan 5 at 4:50
$begingroup$
@summerrain Wrong. Falling at constant rate you will not only not feel lighter but if you were to weigh yourself on a scale your weight will not be lighter. The scale will only register you as lighter when speed changes. Weight is caused by acceleration, not speed. That's why gravity's physical unit is acceleration (m/s/s) not speed (m/s)
$endgroup$
– slebetman
Jan 5 at 4:50
add a comment |
$begingroup$
A few things.
That accident took place at night and in IMC while the aircraft was passing through a storm which I suspect had quite a bit of turbulence in its core. This would most likely have caused enough vestibular disturbance for the crew to make the acceleration towards the deck go unnoticeable.
Secondly the crew was on an IFR flight which means flying by instruments in conditions and specifically trained to rely on cockpit instruments for aircraft orientation and not vestibular senses. Doing otherwise will get you killed in any other situation. Once the aircraft reached its peak descent rate in dark night over water and with the air data sensors iced over, preventing the flight control computers from receiving accurate air data, there was no perceptible means for the crew to sense they were in danger until it was all over.
Pilots do experience the vestibular sense of accelerating as occupants in a carnival ride do - try flying aerobatics some time and yes, it’s quite noticeable when pulling negative Gs, etc. it’s just not a good standard to use to judge movement in an airplane by and without visual references outside of the aircraft it is difficult and very dangerous to use vestibular senses to accurately assess aircraft attitude from.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
A few things.
That accident took place at night and in IMC while the aircraft was passing through a storm which I suspect had quite a bit of turbulence in its core. This would most likely have caused enough vestibular disturbance for the crew to make the acceleration towards the deck go unnoticeable.
Secondly the crew was on an IFR flight which means flying by instruments in conditions and specifically trained to rely on cockpit instruments for aircraft orientation and not vestibular senses. Doing otherwise will get you killed in any other situation. Once the aircraft reached its peak descent rate in dark night over water and with the air data sensors iced over, preventing the flight control computers from receiving accurate air data, there was no perceptible means for the crew to sense they were in danger until it was all over.
Pilots do experience the vestibular sense of accelerating as occupants in a carnival ride do - try flying aerobatics some time and yes, it’s quite noticeable when pulling negative Gs, etc. it’s just not a good standard to use to judge movement in an airplane by and without visual references outside of the aircraft it is difficult and very dangerous to use vestibular senses to accurately assess aircraft attitude from.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
A few things.
That accident took place at night and in IMC while the aircraft was passing through a storm which I suspect had quite a bit of turbulence in its core. This would most likely have caused enough vestibular disturbance for the crew to make the acceleration towards the deck go unnoticeable.
Secondly the crew was on an IFR flight which means flying by instruments in conditions and specifically trained to rely on cockpit instruments for aircraft orientation and not vestibular senses. Doing otherwise will get you killed in any other situation. Once the aircraft reached its peak descent rate in dark night over water and with the air data sensors iced over, preventing the flight control computers from receiving accurate air data, there was no perceptible means for the crew to sense they were in danger until it was all over.
Pilots do experience the vestibular sense of accelerating as occupants in a carnival ride do - try flying aerobatics some time and yes, it’s quite noticeable when pulling negative Gs, etc. it’s just not a good standard to use to judge movement in an airplane by and without visual references outside of the aircraft it is difficult and very dangerous to use vestibular senses to accurately assess aircraft attitude from.
$endgroup$
A few things.
That accident took place at night and in IMC while the aircraft was passing through a storm which I suspect had quite a bit of turbulence in its core. This would most likely have caused enough vestibular disturbance for the crew to make the acceleration towards the deck go unnoticeable.
Secondly the crew was on an IFR flight which means flying by instruments in conditions and specifically trained to rely on cockpit instruments for aircraft orientation and not vestibular senses. Doing otherwise will get you killed in any other situation. Once the aircraft reached its peak descent rate in dark night over water and with the air data sensors iced over, preventing the flight control computers from receiving accurate air data, there was no perceptible means for the crew to sense they were in danger until it was all over.
Pilots do experience the vestibular sense of accelerating as occupants in a carnival ride do - try flying aerobatics some time and yes, it’s quite noticeable when pulling negative Gs, etc. it’s just not a good standard to use to judge movement in an airplane by and without visual references outside of the aircraft it is difficult and very dangerous to use vestibular senses to accurately assess aircraft attitude from.
edited Jan 6 at 16:49
answered Jan 6 at 16:41
Carlo FelicioneCarlo Felicione
43.3k478155
43.3k478155
add a comment |
add a comment |
$begingroup$
Sensation of motion can come through three of the physical senses. Each of the three senses is successively unavailable as we move through the examples of an amusement park ride, the plummeting elevator, and AF447.
Vision
This sense is fairly well understood, so I'll limit this bullet point to pointing out that the visual sense is correlated in the human brain to the vestibular sense, and on the amusement park ride, it is correlated to the vestibular sense. In cases where those two senses are decoupled, it is common for nausea to result. In the elevator and airplane in instrument conditions, a visual sense grounding you to a world reference frame is denied. This is one of the senses which can detect unaccelerated motion, more on "unaccelerated" below.
Touch/Kinesthetic
Here we have information which comes in through our sense of touch, primarily through nerve endings in the skin. The roller coaster comes through here again by exposing you to the elements giving you a "rush of wind" in your face as you move through what may be a stationary mass of air. In the closed environments of the elevator and cockpit, this is again denied.
Vestibular/Inertial
Here is where things get interesting. For the purposes of this discussion, I'll add to the vestibular senses the "seat of the pants", "pit of the stomach" and other bodily means of detecting acceleration. Several other answers have pointed out that all of these senses are sensitive not to velocity but to acceleration, which high school physics reminds us is the the time rate of change, or derivative, of velocity. This means that if velocity is not changing we are not going to have a sensation of motion, although in a gravity field, we may have some sense of orientation (you can have a sense of hanging upside down or laying sideways). There is no difference to these senses between sitting on the ground with the engines running, cruising at 500 knots in level flight, or being in a 500,1000, or 3000 foot per minute descent. As long as velocity is not changing, these senses cannot tell the difference. This is the problem with AF447, No visual, no touch, and in unaccelerated descent, no vestibular sense.
If the airdata instruments are offline, there is nothing to indicate the descent until the cabin pressure starts to increase, putting some pressure on your eardrums. That also won't happen until the aircraft descends to a point where the outside air pressure increases to a point above the cabin pressure then continues to build to a point that you notice. They were doomed.
There is another problem, even with detecting acceleration. The problem is connected to the human brain's remarkable ability to filter out stimuli. A normal human brain is capable of adjusting to the tint of your sunglasses, ignoring conversations in a crowded room, and is very good at not notifying the conscious thought center of the pressure your elastic socks are putting on your ankles. Indeed, it can even lose track of constant accelerations. If you live near a playa, you can do a blind test by driving your car out and enter a gentle turn giving a tenth of a "g" of lateral acceleration. Have someone else do the driving. I am here to tell you that the resulting sensation can be filtered out. Even our sense of acceleration can be lost under constant acceleration. In physics, there is yet another level of derivative, the time rate of change of acceleration, called "jerk", which is harder to not detect.
For more information on the brain's filtering capability, look up the "Reticular Activating System". Understanding it can help you be more safe, and also help you reach your goals.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
Sensation of motion can come through three of the physical senses. Each of the three senses is successively unavailable as we move through the examples of an amusement park ride, the plummeting elevator, and AF447.
Vision
This sense is fairly well understood, so I'll limit this bullet point to pointing out that the visual sense is correlated in the human brain to the vestibular sense, and on the amusement park ride, it is correlated to the vestibular sense. In cases where those two senses are decoupled, it is common for nausea to result. In the elevator and airplane in instrument conditions, a visual sense grounding you to a world reference frame is denied. This is one of the senses which can detect unaccelerated motion, more on "unaccelerated" below.
Touch/Kinesthetic
Here we have information which comes in through our sense of touch, primarily through nerve endings in the skin. The roller coaster comes through here again by exposing you to the elements giving you a "rush of wind" in your face as you move through what may be a stationary mass of air. In the closed environments of the elevator and cockpit, this is again denied.
Vestibular/Inertial
Here is where things get interesting. For the purposes of this discussion, I'll add to the vestibular senses the "seat of the pants", "pit of the stomach" and other bodily means of detecting acceleration. Several other answers have pointed out that all of these senses are sensitive not to velocity but to acceleration, which high school physics reminds us is the the time rate of change, or derivative, of velocity. This means that if velocity is not changing we are not going to have a sensation of motion, although in a gravity field, we may have some sense of orientation (you can have a sense of hanging upside down or laying sideways). There is no difference to these senses between sitting on the ground with the engines running, cruising at 500 knots in level flight, or being in a 500,1000, or 3000 foot per minute descent. As long as velocity is not changing, these senses cannot tell the difference. This is the problem with AF447, No visual, no touch, and in unaccelerated descent, no vestibular sense.
If the airdata instruments are offline, there is nothing to indicate the descent until the cabin pressure starts to increase, putting some pressure on your eardrums. That also won't happen until the aircraft descends to a point where the outside air pressure increases to a point above the cabin pressure then continues to build to a point that you notice. They were doomed.
There is another problem, even with detecting acceleration. The problem is connected to the human brain's remarkable ability to filter out stimuli. A normal human brain is capable of adjusting to the tint of your sunglasses, ignoring conversations in a crowded room, and is very good at not notifying the conscious thought center of the pressure your elastic socks are putting on your ankles. Indeed, it can even lose track of constant accelerations. If you live near a playa, you can do a blind test by driving your car out and enter a gentle turn giving a tenth of a "g" of lateral acceleration. Have someone else do the driving. I am here to tell you that the resulting sensation can be filtered out. Even our sense of acceleration can be lost under constant acceleration. In physics, there is yet another level of derivative, the time rate of change of acceleration, called "jerk", which is harder to not detect.
For more information on the brain's filtering capability, look up the "Reticular Activating System". Understanding it can help you be more safe, and also help you reach your goals.
$endgroup$
add a comment |
$begingroup$
Sensation of motion can come through three of the physical senses. Each of the three senses is successively unavailable as we move through the examples of an amusement park ride, the plummeting elevator, and AF447.
Vision
This sense is fairly well understood, so I'll limit this bullet point to pointing out that the visual sense is correlated in the human brain to the vestibular sense, and on the amusement park ride, it is correlated to the vestibular sense. In cases where those two senses are decoupled, it is common for nausea to result. In the elevator and airplane in instrument conditions, a visual sense grounding you to a world reference frame is denied. This is one of the senses which can detect unaccelerated motion, more on "unaccelerated" below.
Touch/Kinesthetic
Here we have information which comes in through our sense of touch, primarily through nerve endings in the skin. The roller coaster comes through here again by exposing you to the elements giving you a "rush of wind" in your face as you move through what may be a stationary mass of air. In the closed environments of the elevator and cockpit, this is again denied.
Vestibular/Inertial
Here is where things get interesting. For the purposes of this discussion, I'll add to the vestibular senses the "seat of the pants", "pit of the stomach" and other bodily means of detecting acceleration. Several other answers have pointed out that all of these senses are sensitive not to velocity but to acceleration, which high school physics reminds us is the the time rate of change, or derivative, of velocity. This means that if velocity is not changing we are not going to have a sensation of motion, although in a gravity field, we may have some sense of orientation (you can have a sense of hanging upside down or laying sideways). There is no difference to these senses between sitting on the ground with the engines running, cruising at 500 knots in level flight, or being in a 500,1000, or 3000 foot per minute descent. As long as velocity is not changing, these senses cannot tell the difference. This is the problem with AF447, No visual, no touch, and in unaccelerated descent, no vestibular sense.
If the airdata instruments are offline, there is nothing to indicate the descent until the cabin pressure starts to increase, putting some pressure on your eardrums. That also won't happen until the aircraft descends to a point where the outside air pressure increases to a point above the cabin pressure then continues to build to a point that you notice. They were doomed.
There is another problem, even with detecting acceleration. The problem is connected to the human brain's remarkable ability to filter out stimuli. A normal human brain is capable of adjusting to the tint of your sunglasses, ignoring conversations in a crowded room, and is very good at not notifying the conscious thought center of the pressure your elastic socks are putting on your ankles. Indeed, it can even lose track of constant accelerations. If you live near a playa, you can do a blind test by driving your car out and enter a gentle turn giving a tenth of a "g" of lateral acceleration. Have someone else do the driving. I am here to tell you that the resulting sensation can be filtered out. Even our sense of acceleration can be lost under constant acceleration. In physics, there is yet another level of derivative, the time rate of change of acceleration, called "jerk", which is harder to not detect.
For more information on the brain's filtering capability, look up the "Reticular Activating System". Understanding it can help you be more safe, and also help you reach your goals.
$endgroup$
Sensation of motion can come through three of the physical senses. Each of the three senses is successively unavailable as we move through the examples of an amusement park ride, the plummeting elevator, and AF447.
Vision
This sense is fairly well understood, so I'll limit this bullet point to pointing out that the visual sense is correlated in the human brain to the vestibular sense, and on the amusement park ride, it is correlated to the vestibular sense. In cases where those two senses are decoupled, it is common for nausea to result. In the elevator and airplane in instrument conditions, a visual sense grounding you to a world reference frame is denied. This is one of the senses which can detect unaccelerated motion, more on "unaccelerated" below.
Touch/Kinesthetic
Here we have information which comes in through our sense of touch, primarily through nerve endings in the skin. The roller coaster comes through here again by exposing you to the elements giving you a "rush of wind" in your face as you move through what may be a stationary mass of air. In the closed environments of the elevator and cockpit, this is again denied.
Vestibular/Inertial
Here is where things get interesting. For the purposes of this discussion, I'll add to the vestibular senses the "seat of the pants", "pit of the stomach" and other bodily means of detecting acceleration. Several other answers have pointed out that all of these senses are sensitive not to velocity but to acceleration, which high school physics reminds us is the the time rate of change, or derivative, of velocity. This means that if velocity is not changing we are not going to have a sensation of motion, although in a gravity field, we may have some sense of orientation (you can have a sense of hanging upside down or laying sideways). There is no difference to these senses between sitting on the ground with the engines running, cruising at 500 knots in level flight, or being in a 500,1000, or 3000 foot per minute descent. As long as velocity is not changing, these senses cannot tell the difference. This is the problem with AF447, No visual, no touch, and in unaccelerated descent, no vestibular sense.
If the airdata instruments are offline, there is nothing to indicate the descent until the cabin pressure starts to increase, putting some pressure on your eardrums. That also won't happen until the aircraft descends to a point where the outside air pressure increases to a point above the cabin pressure then continues to build to a point that you notice. They were doomed.
There is another problem, even with detecting acceleration. The problem is connected to the human brain's remarkable ability to filter out stimuli. A normal human brain is capable of adjusting to the tint of your sunglasses, ignoring conversations in a crowded room, and is very good at not notifying the conscious thought center of the pressure your elastic socks are putting on your ankles. Indeed, it can even lose track of constant accelerations. If you live near a playa, you can do a blind test by driving your car out and enter a gentle turn giving a tenth of a "g" of lateral acceleration. Have someone else do the driving. I am here to tell you that the resulting sensation can be filtered out. Even our sense of acceleration can be lost under constant acceleration. In physics, there is yet another level of derivative, the time rate of change of acceleration, called "jerk", which is harder to not detect.
For more information on the brain's filtering capability, look up the "Reticular Activating System". Understanding it can help you be more safe, and also help you reach your goals.
answered Jan 7 at 18:22
2NinerRomeo2NinerRomeo
1,1091715
1,1091715
add a comment |
add a comment |
Thanks for contributing an answer to Aviation Stack Exchange!
- Please be sure to answer the question. Provide details and share your research!
But avoid …
- Asking for help, clarification, or responding to other answers.
- Making statements based on opinion; back them up with references or personal experience.
Use MathJax to format equations. MathJax reference.
To learn more, see our tips on writing great answers.
Sign up or log in
StackExchange.ready(function () {
StackExchange.helpers.onClickDraftSave('#login-link');
});
Sign up using Google
Sign up using Facebook
Sign up using Email and Password
Post as a guest
Required, but never shown
StackExchange.ready(
function () {
StackExchange.openid.initPostLogin('.new-post-login', 'https%3a%2f%2faviation.stackexchange.com%2fquestions%2f58700%2fpeople-in-a-rollercoaster-or-falling-elevator-feel-the-rapid-descent-so-why-don%23new-answer', 'question_page');
}
);
Post as a guest
Required, but never shown
Sign up or log in
StackExchange.ready(function () {
StackExchange.helpers.onClickDraftSave('#login-link');
});
Sign up using Google
Sign up using Facebook
Sign up using Email and Password
Post as a guest
Required, but never shown
Sign up or log in
StackExchange.ready(function () {
StackExchange.helpers.onClickDraftSave('#login-link');
});
Sign up using Google
Sign up using Facebook
Sign up using Email and Password
Post as a guest
Required, but never shown
Sign up or log in
StackExchange.ready(function () {
StackExchange.helpers.onClickDraftSave('#login-link');
});
Sign up using Google
Sign up using Facebook
Sign up using Email and Password
Sign up using Google
Sign up using Facebook
Sign up using Email and Password
Post as a guest
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
Required, but never shown
16
$begingroup$
You seem to assume managing an abnormal situation is natural. It actually requires a lot of training to discard feelings and rely only on instrument indications. Pilots have flown inverted without knowing it, or think they are flying inverted, while they are just flying upright. It can happen at any time, to anyone, when external references are lost, and in particular at night. It's called spatial disorientation. The "the head-up illusion" (p7) for instance can give the feeling of pitching up while the aircraft is accelerating down.
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 12:16
9
$begingroup$
Your elevator comparison is biased, the elevator is falling at constant acceleration (compatible with our feeling of gravity), the problems arise when this acceleration either changes in direction, or in magnitude. The rollercoaster example is meaningful: Close your eyes, you won't know in which direction your are currently moving (and you will start to feel very bad).
$endgroup$
– mins
Jan 4 at 15:08
14
$begingroup$
@summerrain I think you're conflating acceleration and velocity. Acceleration is the change in velocity over time. When you're in an object like an elevator, you feel a change in gravity from accelerating up or down. If you go up 100 stories in an elevator, you only feel a change in gravity while the elevator's velocity is changing (ie, starting or stopping). You don't feel anything while it's just moving up or down at a constant velocity.
$endgroup$
– Josh Eller
Jan 4 at 17:59
4
$begingroup$
"AF447's steady cruise flight prior to the stall" -- Have you read the accounts? The aircraft climbed 3000 feet first, while being tossed around by turbulence and by the pilot's overcorrections, then stalled. I've been in a plane that was stalled (for practice), and I've also experienced turbulence; the sensations I recall during the stall were mild compared to some of that turbulence.
$endgroup$
– David K
Jan 4 at 23:34
1
$begingroup$
Fly your pane like a rollercoaster and it will. A ride on the Vomit Comet costs $5k.
$endgroup$
– Mazura
Jan 5 at 6:51